A Blog by Jonathan Low

 

Oct 15, 2012

Your Cheatin' Heart: Has Contemporary Society Embraced Fraud?

Surveys of business executives report that a majority would cheat to get ahead. Documents are falsified, claims exaggerated. Political candidates are caught misstating facts by 'truth squads'- and polls dont change, commentators hardly mention it. Journalists and writers blatantly plagiarise the works of others - and are dismissed, temporarily. Athletes take performance-enhancing drugs, students cheat - even at Harvard - and the response appears to be indifference or accomodation.

The question is whether, as a society, we have simply decided it's all ok. The Chinese do it, the Europeans do it, in affect, everyone else does, so why shouldn't we?

It would be really easy to blame it on the internet. And many people do. Everyone copies and references everything. Who's to say what's plagiarism and what's legitimately riffing on a common theme?

But the widespread nature of the behavior raises questions about our definition of truth and the trust that goes with it.

The following article reports how this activity has infiltrated what has been, until recently, an unassailable bastion of purity; scientific research. Because of the quantitative nature of the evidence, the rigid peer-review processes and the potential for life-and-death implications, it has always been assumed that no one would dare to try to cheat, let alone get away with it. However, it appears that fraudulent findings are not uncommon. And that the perpetrators are sometimes permitted to withdraw the offending work or otherwise given face-saving solutions.

While admittedly one small and relatively obscure corner of a much larger intellectual and emotional structure, this does suggest that as a society we must come to terms with the sanctity of our relationships - or concede that trust no longer has a useful place in our interactions. JL

Zoe Corbyn reports in Scientific American:
Conventional wisdom says that most retractions of papers in scientific journals are triggered by unintentional errors. Not so, according to one of the largest-ever studies of retractions. A survey published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences has found that two-thirds of retracted life-sciences papers were stricken from the scientific record because of misconduct such as fraud or suspected fraud — and that journals sometimes soft-pedal the reason.

The survey examined all 2,047 articles in the PubMed database that had been marked as retracted by 3 May this year. But rather than taking journals’ retraction notices at face value, as previous analyses have done, the study used secondary sources to pin down the reasons for retraction if the notices were incomplete or vague. These sources included investigations by the US Office of Research Integrity, and evidence reported by the blog Retraction Watch.

The analysis revealed that fraud or suspected fraud was responsible for 43% of the retractions. Other types of misconduct — duplicate publication and plagiarism — accounted for 14% and 10% of retractions, respectively. Only 21% of the papers were retracted because of error.

Earlier studies had found that the percentage of retractions attributable to error was 1.5–3 times higher. “The secondary sources give a very different picture,” says Arturo Casadevall, a microbiologist at Yeshiva University in New York, and a co-author of the latest study. “Retraction notices are often not accurate.”

Elizabeth Wager, a UK-based medical writer and co-author of a previous study that relied on journal retraction notices, isn’t surprised by the finding of hidden misconduct. “We found many notices that seemed deliberately obscure or vague,” she says, speculating that authors and journals may use opaque retraction notices to save face or avoid libel charges.

The latest study shows a ten-fold increase (to about 0.01%) in the proportion of papers retracted owing to fraud since 1975. Previous analyzes have seen a growing trend in retractions in general, but the latest report sheds new light on the extent to which fraud is responsible. It also found a correlation between journal impact factor and the number of fraud-induced retractions, says Ferric Fang, a microbiologist at the University of Washington in Seattle, who led the study.

Influential journals, including Science, Nature, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences and Cell, all appear in the top-ten list of publications with retractions because of fraud or suspected fraud (see ‘Top ten retractors’). For some journals, including the two topping the table — The Journal of Biological Chemistry and Anesthesia & Analgesia — the tally was boosted by multiple retractions from the same few individuals, such as anesthesiologist Joachim Boldt, formerly of the Ludwigshafen Clinical Center in Germany. Indeed, Fang and his colleagues found that 38 research groups with five or more retractions accounted for 44% of articles linked to fraud or suspected fraud.

Whether the overall rise in fraud-induced retractions is the result of an increase in misconduct, or simply down to more scrutiny, is an open question, says Fang. It is also unclear whether the high-impact journals have more retractions for fraud because they are checked more closely, or because they are more likely to attract fraudsters. But Fang thinks that the large rewards for publishing in leading journals — which can range from winning grants to receiving tenure — are powerful incentives that could be driving some of the trend. “We need to look at how we have structured the system, so scientists are not given incentives to [commit fraud] quite as strongly,” he says.

The survey found some significant geographical differences. Retracted papers with lead authors based in historical scientific superpowers, such as the United States and Germany, were more likely to be linked to fraud. In emerging scientific powers such as India and China, however, plagiarism and duplication caused more of the retractions. “These trends may reflect differences in incentives, cultural norms and proficiency in English among these countries,” says Fang.

Ivan Oransky, a New York-based journalist and co-founder of Retraction Watch, suggests setting up a ‘transparency index’ for journals, to rank them on criteria such as the clarity of their retraction notices. The idea, which he says he would be keen to work on, could provide a much-needed incentive for journals to improve their performance in this area. Data from the current study could also serve as a basis for a retractions database to help scientists avoid wasting time trying to replicate or build on retracted work, he adds.

“I’m not necessarily opposed to the idea, but I have concerns about how such a database could be properly maintained and updated,” says Fang. “Our study is merely a snapshot. Creating an accurate, centralized database that could be used as an ongoing resource would be a considerable undertaking.”

0 comments:

Post a Comment